[Home] [Headlines] [Latest Articles] [Latest Comments] [Post] [Mail] [Sign-in] [Setup] [Help] [Register]
Status: Not Logged In; Sign In
Science-Technology Title: DELINGPOLE: ‘Penises Cause Climate Change’; Progressives Fooled By Peer-Reviewed Hoax Study Gender studies is a fake academic industry populated by charlatans, deranged activists and gullible idiots. Now, a pair of enterprising hoaxers has proved it scientifically by persuading an academic journal to peer-review and publish their paper claiming that the penis is not really a male genital organ but a social construct. The paper, published by Cogent Social Sciences a multidisciplinary open access journal offering high quality peer review across the social sciences also claims that penises are responsible for causing climate change. The two hoaxers are Peter Boghossian, a full-time faculty member in the Philosophy department at Portland State University, and James Lindsay, who has a doctorate in math and a background in physics. They were hoping to emulate probably the most famous academic hoax in recent years: the Sokal Hoax named after NYU and UCL physics professor Alan Sokal who in 1996 persuaded an academic journal called Social Text to accept a paper titled Transgressing the Boundaries: Towards a Transformative Hermeneutics of Quantum Gravity. Sokals paper comprising pages of impressive-sounding but meaningless pseudo-academic jargon was written in part to demonstrate that humanities journals will publish pretty much anything so long as it sounds like proper leftist thought; and partly in order to send up the absurdity of so much post-modernist social science. So, for this new spoof, Boghossian and Lindsay were careful to throw in lots of signifier phrases to indicate fashionable anti-male bias: Sokals paper comprising pages of impressive-sounding but meaningless pseudo-academic jargon was written in part to demonstrate that humanities journals will publish pretty much anything so long as it sounds like proper leftist thought; and partly in order to send up the absurdity of so much post-modernist social science. So, for this new spoof, Boghossian and Lindsay were careful to throw in lots of signifier phrases to indicate fashionable anti-male bias: We intended to test the hypothesis that flattery of the academic Lefts moral architecture in general, and of the moral orthodoxy in gender studies in particular, is the overwhelming determiner of publication in an academic journal in the field. That is, we sought to demonstrate that a desire for a certain moral view of the world to be validated could overcome the critical assessment required for legitimate scholarship. Particularly, we suspected that gender studies is crippled academically by an overriding almost-religious belief that maleness is the root of all evil. On the evidence, our suspicion was justified. They also took care to make it completely incomprehensible. We didnt try to make the paper coherent; instead, we stuffed it full of jargon (like discursive and isomorphism), nonsense (like arguing that hypermasculine men are both inside and outside of certain discourses at the same time), red-flag phrases (like pre-post-patriarchal society), lewd references to slang terms for the penis, insulting phrasing regarding men (including referring to some men who choose not to have children as being unable to coerce a mate), and allusions to rape (we stated that manspreading, a complaint levied against men for sitting with their legs spread wide, is akin to raping the empty space around him). After completing the paper, we read it carefully to ensure it didnt say anything meaningful, and as neither one of us could determine what it is actually about, we deemed it a success. Some of it was written with the help of the Postmodern Generator a website coded in the 1990s by Andrew Bulhak featuring an algorithm, based on NYU physicist Alan Sokals method of hoaxing a cultural studies journal called Social Text, that returns a different fake postmodern paper every time the page is reloaded. This paragraph, for example, looks impressive but is literally meaningless: Inasmuch as masculinity is essentially performative, so too is the conceptual penis. The penis, in the words of Judith Butler, can only be understood through reference to what is barred from the signifier within the domain of corporeal legibility (Butler, 1993). The penis should not be understood as an honest expression of the performers intent should it be presented in a performance of masculinity or hypermasculinity. Thus, the isomorphism between the conceptual penis and whats referred to throughout discursive feminist literature as toxic hypermasculinity, is one defined upon a vector of male cultural machismo braggadocio, with the conceptual penis playing the roles of subject, object, and verb of action. The result of this trichotomy of roles is to place hypermasculine men both within and outside of competing discourses whose dynamics, as seen via post-structuralist discourse analysis, enact a systematic interplay of power in which hypermasculine men use the conceptual penis to move themselves from powerless subject positions to powerful ones (confer: Foucault, 1972). None of it should have survived more than a moments scrutiny by serious academics. But it was peer-reviewed by two experts in the field who, after suggesting only a few changes, passed it for publication: Cogent Social Sciences eventually accepted The Conceptual Penis as a Social Construct. The reviewers were amazingly encouraging, giving us very high marks in nearly every category. For example, one reviewer graded our thesis statement sound and praised it thusly, It capturs [sic] the issue of hypermasculinity through a multi-dimensional and nonlinear process (which we take to mean that it wanders aimlessly through many layers of jargon and nonsense). The other reviewer marked the thesis, along with the entire paper, outstanding in every applicable category. They didnt accept the paper outright, however. Cogent Social Sciences Reviewer #2 offered us a few relatively easy fixes to make our paper better. We effortlessly completed them in about two hours, putting in a little more nonsense about manspreading (which we alleged to be a cause of climate change) and dick-measuring contests. No claim made in the paper was considered too ludicrous by the peer-reviewers: not even the one claiming that the penis is the universal performative source of rape, and is the conceptual driver behind much of climate change. You read that right. We argued that climate change is conceptually caused by penises. How do we defend that assertion? Like this: Destructive, unsustainable hegemonically male approaches to pressing environmental policy and action are the predictable results of a raping of nature by a male-dominated mindset. This mindset is best captured by recognizing the role of [sic] the conceptual penis holds over masculine psychology. When it is applied to our natural environment, especially virgin environments that can be cheaply despoiled for their material resources and left dilapidated and diminished when our patriarchal approaches to economic gain have stolen their inherent worth, the extrapolation of the rape culture inherent in the conceptual penis becomes clear. The fact that such complete drivel was published in a social science journal, the hoaxers argue, raises serious questions about the value of fields like gender studies and the state of academic publishing generally: The Conceptual Penis as a Social Construct should not have been published on its merits because it was actively written to avoid having any merits whatsoever. The paper is academically worthless nonsense. But they do not hold out much hope for it having any more effect on the bullshit in the social sciences industry than Sokals hoax did because leftist stupidity in academe is so heavily entrenched. As a matter of deeper concern, there is unfortunately some reason to believe that our hoax will not break the relevant spell. First, Alan Sokals hoax, now more than 20 years old, did not prevent the continuation of bizarre postmodernist scholarship. In particular, it did not lead to a general tightening of standards that would have blocked our own hoax. Second, people rarely give up on their moral attachments and ideological commitments just because theyre shown to be out of alignment with reality. Post Comment Private Reply Ignore Thread Top Page Up Full Thread Page Down Bottom/Latest
#1. To: cranky, Deckard (#0)
I post these same kinds of fake science exposes. I'm not sure it tells us much except there are a lot of very marginal small science publishing houses that have no credibility. Still, they are supposedly subject to replicability and to proper vetting by qualified reviewers so it does serve to impeach their integrity. I tend to think that even if a hoaxer drives one of these con job science publishers out of business completely, they would just re-open under a new name and start over in the same racket. I posted one recently you might not have noticed. LF: Journal Trolled With Study About a Fake Disease From "Seinfeld"
It tells me whether or not a paper is 'peer reviewed' is a distinction without a difference.
"The Endochronic Properties of Resublimated Thiotimoline"
If Seinfeld didn't have an episode about it, I'm completely lost.
Perhaps this theory is right? Sounds better than Al Gore Nobel Price recipient theory.
I wonder why climate deniers don't have to pay?
Another great find Cranky. Lol.
I thought you were a Sci-Fi fan, no?
The "drivel" is not really drivel. Nor is it nonsense. Perhaps it is nonsense to the authors - who wrote what they do not believe. And perhaps it is nonsense in the absolute, meaning that it is literally not true. But the sentences do hold together, and they do convey a coherent series of thoughts. There is quite a bit of coherent logic in the argument, in the sense that the conclusions flow from the assumptions. What is missing is any sort of factual basis for the assumptions. It isn't right to call the paper incoherent and meaningless. It is coherent, and its conclusion logically flows from its premises. The problem with the paper is that it is devoid of facts to prove any of the assertions on which the logic hangs. It was, in assence an argument predicated upon a series of assertions that were neither proven nor demonstrated. The paper reviewers should have demanded data to back the assertions. They did not err in following the logic - because the logic itself does follow. It's not "drivel", it's not incoherent. It's an opinion piece based on assumed and unsupported facts.
You really did not get the joke, did you?
I did get the joke, yes.
It's not "drivel", it's not incoherent. It's an opinion piece based on assumed and unsupported facts. That's the overall problem. The reviewers believed their own press. Meaning they recognized terms and logical arguments they frequently use and instead of looking for corroborating evidence saw the study as affirmations of their held beliefs. We should not be surprised as we see this in theology all too often. People quoting other theologians as "fact" without digging into how they came to their conclusions.
Yes. Exactly. You understood me. I don't agree with anything in the paper. I think the ideas are foolish. But it is not true that the article is incoherent drivel. They didn't just string buzzwords together and produce a jumble. There was an argument there, it wasn't "nonsense" in the truest meaning of the word. There was sense there, argument, direction, a point, a purpose. What there was not, was data to back the assertions. It was a series of explosive assertions ending in a conclusion that logically followed. The logic was there, the facts were not.
|
[Home] [Headlines] [Latest Articles] [Latest Comments] [Post] [Mail] [Sign-in] [Setup] [Help] [Register]
|